Open Access
4 May 2012 Load-dependent brain activation assessed by time-domain functional near-infrared spectroscopy during a working memory task with graded levels of difficulty
Author Affiliations +
Abstract
We evaluated frontal brain activation during a mixed attentional/working memory task with graded levels of difficulty in a group of 19 healthy subjects, by means of time-domain functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Brain activation was assessed, and load-related oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin changes were studied. Generalized linear model (GLM) was applied to the data to explore the metabolic processes occurring during the mental effort and, possibly, their involvement in short-term memorization. GLM was applied to the data twice: for modeling the task as a whole and for specifically investigating brain activation at each cognitive load. This twofold employment of GLM allowed (1) the extraction and isolation of different information from the same signals, obtained through the modeling of different cognitive categories (sustained attention and working memory), and (2) the evaluation of model fitness, by inspection and comparison of residuals (i.e., unmodeled part of the signal) obtained in the two different cases. Results attest to the presence of a persistent attentional-related metabolic activity, superimposed to a task-related mnemonic contribution. Some hemispherical differences have also been highlighted frontally: deoxy-hemoglobin changes manifested a strong right lateralization, whereas modifications in oxy- and total hemoglobin showed a medial localization. The present work successfully explored the capability of fNIRS to detect the two neurophysiological categories under investigation and distinguish their activation patterns.

1.

Introduction

Working memory (WM) is the cognitive function resulting from the active short-term memory system that guides and controls humans’ behavior. It allows the temporary storage and the further recollecting of information.1 Importantly, it is a limited-capacity resource, which thus can easily saturate under extremely stressful conditions, with excessively rapid streams of information, and in disproportionately complex environmental settings.

Psychophysiology has demonstrated sustained activity in the frontal and parietal brain areas while information is held in mind.24 Visual WM, specifically, involves the concerted activity of a distributed neural system, including anterior areas in prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the visual cortex posteriorly.5 Within the visual cortex, ventral stream areas are selectively involved in object vision, whereas dorsal stream areas are involved in spatial vision.6 This domain specificity appears to extend forward into PFC, with ventrolateral areas involved mainly in WM for objects and dorsolateral areas involved mainly in WM for spatial locations.2,4,7

Moreover, interactions between the inferior temporal neocortex, PFC, and parietal cortex likely support WM maintenance via an attention-based mechanism, enhancing the relevant information among irrelevant background noise.8,9 Overlap of the cerebral networks of WM and attention has been demonstrated in targeted comparisons.10 Specifically, superimposition of activation was observed in the frontal midline and in a part of the right and inferior frontal gyrus in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies.11 As a result, competition for processing resources that are shared by the WM and attention systems can lead to a severe limitation of neural processing capabilities. One major bottleneck of information processing, indeed, arises from the common demands on neural resources shared between visual WM and attention during the encoding stage.

Some regions in the left PFC and in the inferior temporal cortex have shown selective responsiveness to cognitive load during a WM task.11 Areas selectively responsive to high attentional demand have been highlighted by Mayer et al.11 within the right prefrontal and bilateral occipital cortex. Consequently, they inferred a hemispheric specialization, with left PFC selectively responsive to WM load and right PFC selectively responsive to attentional demand. On the other hand, some evidence suggests that the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and the PFC serve complementary roles for WM processes: whereas the PFC appears to be predominantly important for WM maintenance of familiar items such as words or numbers, the MTL may play a role for maintenance of unfamiliar novel items.12 In addition, it has been shown that multi-item memory processing depends on the organization of item representations by the same MTL.12 Rissman et al.13 have put forward that two neural circuits may dynamically trade off to accommodate the particular mnemonic demands of the task: some results credit the establishment of interactions between frontal gyrus and fusiform face area, mediating maintenance of information at lower cognitive loads, and the involvement of hippocampus, supporting the retention when a higher effort is needed. In addition, Jaeggi et al.14 have specifically investigated the modulation effect induced by the mnemonic demand of the task on the activation in the PFC. By showing that activation increases with memory load in the PFC, even when the capacity of the WM’s central executive system is exceeded, they definitively uncoupled neural activation from behavioral accuracy measures and performance.

Areas of high neuronal activation show increased oxygen consumption and enhanced blood supply to ensure provision of oxygenated hemoglobin.9,15 As a consequence, neural activation can be detected by measuring changes of regional hemoglobin in cerebral blood. Since it development in 1977,16 functional near-infrared spectr1oscopy (fNIRS) has been increasingly employed as a neuroimaging technique: fNIRS allows for the noninvasive monitoring of temporal and spatial changes in oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin concentrations (O2Hb and HHb, respectively),1721 thus providing indexes of brain activation. In particular, multichannel fNIRS can measure cortical activation during cognitive processing without interfering with task procedure; indeed, it has been successfully employed in protocols evaluating psychological and emotional pressure22 and in pediatric studies.23

Schreppel et al.24 investigated the effects of interference resolution processes on activation of the PFC in a WM task by means of fNIRS. Shibuya-Tayoshi et al.25 and Nakahachi et al.26 employed fNIRS devices to obtain a mapping of the activation of the PFC during the visuospatial WM Trail Making Test. They reported bilateral activation, primarily in the PFC.

Lee et al.27 examined qualitatively distinct types of error trials during the execution of a delayed WM task to further specify the behavioral and neural difference between errors that arise from a loss of mental representation versus those that arise from the encoding of an incorrect stimulus; they applied both fNIRS and fMRI for a comparison of activations. To our knowledge, though, no study has investigated the capabilities of fNIRS devices to detect hemodynamic changes due to variation in cognitive load. Although fMRI and positron emission tomographic (PET) studies have fathomed the matter,14,2831 the applicability of easy-to-use, noninvasive optical instrumentation could open the investigation to much broader cohorts.

Last, the fNIRS technique has been employed for the investigation of WM abnormalities in a number of psychiatric cohorts, ranging from patients with major depressive disorder,32 to those with schizophrenia33 and children showing attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.34

The present study mainly introduces two items of novelty with respect to the literature mentioned above: (1) the employment of NIRS devices in the investigation of the modulatory effect arisen in the frontal hemodynamic pattern by means of stimulation with differential cognitive load and (2) the use of time-domain fNIRS (based on high-repetition-rate light sources and picosecond detection of photon time-of-flight) to improve depth sensitivity to cortical hemodynamic changes.3538

The aims of this work were (1) to determine whether multichannel time-domain fNIRS is able to discern differences in frontal patterns of activation due to pure attentional and WM tasks and (2) to assess time-domain fNIRS sensitivity in detecting modulation effects induced by the mnemonic demand of a cognitive task with graded levels of difficulty. This was done by focusing on frontal cortical activation during an n-back task with different stages of memory requirement.

2.

Materials and Methods

2.1.

Time-Domain fNIRS

Two pulsed diode lasers, operating at frequencies of 690 and 820 nm, with 80 MHz repetition rate and 1 mW average power (PDL, Picoquant GmbH, Germany), were used as light sources. The laser heads were connected to multimode graded index fibers (50/125μm) by means of a homemade coupler that combines a neutral density attenuator (J54-082, Edmund OptiK GmbH, Germany), with variable attenuation in the range 0 to 80 dB, and a standard FC fiber optics coupler. Multimode graded index optical fibers (50/125μm) with different lengths and a 2-by-2 fused fiber optic splitter (VISNIR5050, OZ Optics, Canada) were used to time multiplexing of the laser pulses at the different wavelengths and to create two independent channels. In each channel, a 1-by-9 fiber optic switch (F-SM19, PiezoJena GmbH, Germany) created up to nine independent sources or injection points; therefore 18 sources were available.

Four parallel detection chains accomplished acquisition of time-resolved reflectance curves. The parallel use of the four detection and acquisition lines enabled a total of 16 independent detectors. Fiber optic bundles, 1.5 m length (Loptek GmbH, Germany), were used for light collection from human tissue.

The system was controlled by a personal computer (Pentium IV 3.5 GHz, 2 Gb RAM), which hosted the acquisition boards and stored data. Personalized software, written in C language, was used to control the instrument. The software was interfaced to a microcontroller unit (dsPIC30F6014, Microchip Technology, AZ), which was used for the hardware control of the instrumentation. The microcontroller unit generated trigger signals for the synchronization of data acquisition by the TCSPC boards and of the sequential activation of the sources. The instrument response function (IRF) obtained by filling all the propagating modes of the bundle had a FWHM of approximately 500 ps. A detailed description and characterization of the system can be found in Contini et al.39

The system also interfaced with dedicated software (Presentation, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA) for precise stimulus delivery and experimental control program.

For each wavelength λ, a reference time-domain fNIRS curve R0(τ,λ) was derived by averaging the tracks recorded during the initial baseline period. Fitting of R0(τ,λ) yielded the reference absorption value μa0(λ). Then, at each recording time τ˜ during the experiment, changes in the absorption coefficient were derived40 as

Eq. (1)

Δμa(λ,τ˜)=1vτln[R(τ,λ,τ˜)R0(τ,λ)],
where v is the speed of light in the medium, τ is the arrival time of photons, and R(τ,λ,τ˜) is the time-domain fNIRS curve at the recording time τ˜. To enhance the contribution from deep layers and remove possible disturbances caused by superficial layers, a correction method based on the use of late time windows (τ=1750to2500ps) was also applied.41,42 It is well known that depth information in time-domain fNIRS is encoded in the time-of-flight of photons:37,38,43 early photons probe the superficial layers of the head, whereas late photons have a higher probability to visit deeper layers. Finally, the absorption coefficient was then derived from corrected late gate intensities as

Eq. (2)

μa(λ,τ˜)=μa0(λ)+Δμa(λ,τ˜).
Taking the assumption that, in the wavelengths applied here, O2Hb, HHb, and H2O are the main chromophores contributing to absorption, and also assuming that H2O concentration is unlikely to be subject to relevant modifications and thus can be considered constant, O2Hb and HHb concentration changes were derived by Lambert-Beer law. Then, changes in total hemoglobin content (τHb=HHb+O2Hb) were calculated.

Finally, low-frequency and high-frequency components were extracted from the hemodynamic data by use of a digital filter (ninth-order digital Chebyshev filter, Matlab) with the stop-band 30 dB down. Transition frequency was selected as the inverse of the period of the task modulation.

2.2.

Optodes Localization

The optical probe was placed over the head to cover the underlying prefrontal cortex, and light sources were centered (according to the international 10 to 20 system for the EEG electrode placement) at the Fp1 and Fp2 for left and right sides, respectively (Fig. 1). The source-detector distance was 2 cm, in accordance with previous experiments.44 It should also be considered that, in time-resolved fNIRS, the capability of photons to penetrate brain tissue does not depend on the interfiber distance.43 No downward sliding of the fiber holders was observed at the end of the measurements in any subject. Custom-made fiber holders were designed to keep fibers on the forehead with black rubber pads. Holders were kept in place on the head with two-sided tape and velcro bands. The minimum distance between the light source located on the left (right) forehead and detectors placed over the right (left) forehead was 5 cm. This distance was chosen after a trial, performed with both phantoms and in vivo measurements, aiming to rule out cross-talk between optical signals detected simultaneously in the two areas.

Fig. 1

Position of fNIRS probes (injection sources in red, collection bundles in green) on the subject’s head.

JBO_17_5_056005_f001.png

2.3.

Statistics

To examine whether there were statistically significant changes in fNIRS signals, amplitude analysis on the O2Hb, HHb, and tHb measures was performed. A t-test was conducted for assessing differences between fNIRS data acquired over the two hemispheres (right versus left). Then, repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted, with two within-group factors: condition (rest and loads) and channel. Post-hoc analyses were performed with pairwise ANOVAs. A P value of 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. The Greenhouse-Geisser procedure was used to correct for sphericity violations, where necessary.

t-Tests were also conducted on behavioral data, to assess differences in performance between conditions. All the statistical analyses were performed with NCSS Statistical Software (NCSS Statistical & Power Analysis Software, http://www.ncss.com).

2.4.

Generalized Linear Model

Both low- and high-frequency components of O2Hb, HHb, and tHb time courses (time series) were analyzed with the generalized linear model (GLM), which is a well-known statistical method suitable for analyzing time series data.45 The GLM approach has been extensively applied in fMRI data analysis4649 and has been introduced for analysis of fNIRS data.5054

A time series for a given channel was modeled as a linear combination of L regressors (known functions) plus an error term:

Eq. (3)

Y(τ×1)=X(τ×L)β(τ×1)+ε(τ×1),
where X (τ×L matrix) is called the design matrix (DM), each column is a regressor, and τ is the number of time points. Each column L of matrix X contains the predicted hemodynamic response for one experimental block over time τ. β is the vector of the unknown parameters, one for each regressor, weighting (i.e., quantifying) the contribution of each predictor (i.e., regressor) for modeling the functional time series (i.e., columns of matrix Y), and it serves as the parameter set for subsequent hypothesis testing. Errors ε are assumed to have normal distribution with zero mean and covariance σ2V, where σ2 represents variances and V is the temporal correlation matrix (identical at all voxels).

Origin for correlation on error terms can be found in cardiac, respiratory, and vasomotor effects. The ordinary least squares estimation of the parameters is

Eq. (4)

β^=(Xτ˜X)1Xτ˜Y.
Inference is then performed by testing the predictor variables through the t-statistic:

Eq. (5)

t=X^(r)CX^(r),
where X^(r) is the product of the transposed contrast vector with the response signal strength and CX^(r) is the error variance.

The interest of this work was the analysis of the mean level of brain activation, deducible from levels of O2Hb and HHb, and of their time course. The task was modeled as a series of consecutive boxcar regressors, each one marking with a unit value a single condition (e.g., two activation periods with the same cognitive load) and with a zero value all the remaining part of the task. The timing of the regressors was chosen according to the different activities performed during the experiment. The GLM was applied separately for O2Hb, HHb, and tHb data. The hemodynamic response function was the same for O2Hb, HHb, and tHb data.54 First-level analysis (i.e., on single subjects) was performed first. Activated areas were extracted by setting uncorrected P value of the t-statistics at 0.05.

Inferential procedures at a second level (e.g., about activated channels over all subjects) were then performed, by setting uncorrected P value of the t-statistics at 0.001, and the DM was accordingly modified.

Contrast matrix was designed for a direct comparison of (1) activation during the two task periods and rest condition with respect to the reference (whole test) [see Fig. 2(a)], (2) activation during each cognitive load and rest condition with respect to the load/rest immediately harder/easier [see Fig. 2(b)], and (3) activation during each cognitive load and rest condition with respect to the reference (whole test) [see Fig. 2(b)].

Fig. 2

Design matrix DM1, modeling baseline, task, and rest (a); time course proceeds from top to bottom, and baseline, task, rest, and reference functions are aligned from left to right. Design matrix DM2, modeling baseline, the four cognitive loads, and rest (b): time course proceeds from top to bottom, and baseline, 0-, 1-, 2-, and 3-back, rest, and reference functions are aligned from left to right.

JBO_17_5_056005_f002.png

Cerebral activation was indicated by positive t values in the O2Hb and tHb time series, and by negative t values for HHb. The results from these second-level analyses were plotted as statistical parametric maps in NIRS_SPM,54 which illustrate the brain regions where increased or decreased O2Hb, HHb, and tHb correlate with the stimulation protocol over time.

As shown in Butti et al.,44 Pearson product moment correlation coefficient at lag 1 of basal fNIRS signals is not significantly different from zero for both O2Hb and HHb series, if the P value is set at 0.05. Thus, no corrections for serial correlations were applied in the GLM.

2.5.

Activation Task

Each participant was given a computerized variant of the n-back task, using letters of the alphabet as memoranda.55 The n-back task is a verbal memory test that provides four levels of memory load (0-, 1-, 2-, and 3-back), presented in a factorial design. It yields a total of eight task blocks (four conditions, repeated twice). For each difficulty modality, computerized lists of 30 stimuli were constructed and then presented in a pseudorandom order, making up a total of 240 intermingled letters shown to the subject. A block of a single condition lasted 67 s, consisting of 5-s presentation of task instructions (e.g., “2-back letter task”), followed by a 2-s pause and then 30 stimuli (250-ms presentation of a stimulus, followed by a 1750-ms blank-screen interstimulus interval [ISI]). Each block of a single condition was presented separately, and between the blocks a rest of 30 s was given. A 60-s baseline and 60-s recovery were acquired at the start and at the end of the test, for a total acquisition time of 16 min.

At the end of the first four blocks (0-, 2-, 1-, and 3-back), in the middle of the test, subjects saw the word REST for approximately 90 s before the same four blocks, in different order (2-, 1-, 3-, and 0-back) began, providing a supplementary rest period. On each trial, subjects observed stimuli presented in the center of a computer screen (Presentation software, Neurobehavioral Systems). Letters were presented in 24-point Helvetica. Subjects responded to each stimulus presentation by pressing the left or right button on the mouse in their right hand. To respond to a stimulus as a target, subjects pressed the button under their index finger (left button); to respond to a stimulus as a nontarget, they pressed the button under their middle finger (right button).56

In the 0-back condition, the target letter was any letter that matched the one specified during the instructions given at the beginning of the block (in this test, letter “A”). In the 1-back condition, the target was any stimulus identical to the immediately preceding stimulus. In the 2-back and 3-back conditions, the target was any stimulus identical to the stimulus presented two or three trials prior, respectively (Fig. 3). Stimuli were targets on 20% of trials, at all levels of load (every condition).

Fig. 3

The n-back test for attention and working-memory assessment. 0-back condition allows the evaluation of pure attention; higher loads require increasing working memory resources for both delay and recall.

JBO_17_5_056005_f003.png

Subject performance during recording was monitored in terms of reaction time and accuracy (number of target letters identified correctly). Any correct reaction to the stimulus (both target and nontarget) was a commission; any incorrect answer to the stimulus was a commission error. Any missing response (to both target and nontarget stimuli) was an omission error.

Before beginning the experimental task, all participants were trained on the task and were given a pretesting session to ensure that they had satisfactory understanding of the instructions.

It is well established that the n-back task utilizes executive functions,56and that these functions are located in prefrontal cortex.57

2.6.

Subjects

Nineteen healthy volunteers, comparable for age (mean 27.15 years, SD 2.01, range 23 to 30) and school attendance (mean 19.9 years, SD 1.2, range 17 to 21), participated in the experiment. Seventeen subjects were right-handed. All volunteers were native Italian speakers and were not paid to participate. The neuropsychological status of each participant was assessed by means of an unpublished adapted Italian version of the Neuropsychological History Questionnaire.58 They all had normal vision and had no history of psychiatric disorders. None of them showed any neuropsychological illness; cognitive level and attentive capability were normal; none had any first-degree relatives with a psychiatric illness. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all volunteers after the examination and test procedure had been explained.

3.

Results

3.1.

Behavioral Results

The 19 subjects committed an average of 1.58 errors (SD 0.69) in the n-back task, thus falling in the normal range.55 On average, they committed 0.77 errors during the 0-back condition, 1.28 errors during the 1-back, 2.14 during the 2-back, and 2.15 during the 3-back. They answered correctly to the target in 511 ms (SD 36). More specifically, reaction times on targets were 464 ms for the 0-back condition, 508 for the 1-back, 540 ms for the 2-back, and 535 ms for the 3-back.

A statistically significant difference was found for errors committed at the four different cognitive loads (P<0.05), as well as for reaction times, grouped for the same four loads (P<0.05).

3.2.

fNIRS Results

Results emerging from time-domain fNIRS acquisitions show that variations of O2Hb remained positive in the PFC, over both hemispheres and during the entire test (Fig. 4). Activation level increased during the first half of the test, reaching a peak in the central part of it; then, activation remained stable during the second part of the task, with a final and further increase in the last recovery period. In addition to the large, persistent changes described hitherto, transient and less prominent changes were observed time-locked to the active blocks presentation.

Fig. 4

Time course of O2Hb (red line, circle) and HHb (blue line, triangle). Averages (thick line) and standard deviations (thin line) over the population of the study (all channels). O2Hb increased during the first half of the test, and its values remained high; HHb values tended to decrease, reaching a stable equilibrium very early during the test.

JBO_17_5_056005_f004.png

Variations of HHb globally stayed in a range of negative values throughout the test. Right channels showed an initial decrease and an ensuing increase in value, which moved toward the restoration of the rest value; left channels exhibited an initial decrease of HHb as well, with a delayed recovery.

Globally, larger variations of O2Hb and HHb were found over the right frontal area, with respect to the contralateral hemisphere. This hemispheric lateralization, visually discerned on raw data, was later confirmed by statistical analysis: t-test confirmed the statistical difference between the average signals from right and left channels for both O2Hb and HHb. The prevalence of activation on the right side was then assessed, and the right PFC showed higher O2Hb increases than the left one.

Moreover, repeated-measures ANOVA conducted on HHb revealed a strong effect of time (P<0.001) and time-by-channel interaction (P<0.001), while a main effect of channel was not observed (P>0.05). The effect of time is paramount: the task generated a strong hemodynamic response throughout its duration, presenting a momentary decrease only in correspondence with the central rest period. Standard deviation increased with time within both parts of the task, thus testifying to a greater uniformity of behavior among subjects during the “early” response to the task (and then a more pronounced intersubject variability for the long-term response). Repeated-measures ANOVA conducted on O2Hb attested to a significant main effect of time and time-by-channel interaction (P<0.001). Effect of channel was not statistically significant (P>0.05).

To tentatively discriminate between the tonic contribution to activation, due to subject’s involvement in the cognitive challenge, and the phasic cerebral activity, which is more specifically linked to the working memory effort, low- and high-frequency components were extracted from the fNIRS signals. The two contributions were separately considered, with reference to the performance of the cognitive task (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5

(a) Time course of high-frequency and (b) low-frequency contributions of O2Hb (red line, circles) and HHb (blue line, triangles) during the working memory task. As an example, signals belonging to subject 8 are shown. Channels are averaged.

JBO_17_5_056005_f005.png

As mentioned in the previous section, high-frequency components of the signals were first analyzed by means of DM1 [Fig. 2(a)], and then statistical τ-maps were calculated for the analyzed group of volunteers using the t-value parameters. These interpolated maps were obtained through the resolution of the complete GLM at group level. Each map represents the distribution associated to a specific contrast between two regressors. Response of the PFC to task presentation is given in Fig. 6(a) for O2Hb, HHb, and tHb. The temporal trend noted above is confirmed by these activity maps. The task originated a strong metabolic response, leading to an increasing difference between parameter values registered during the test and during the baseline period. The test, indeed, was characterized by a strong and steady activation. Unlike the initial baseline period, the interleaved and final recovery periods could not provide any significant deactivation, if compared to the task. Modifications were spread over the PFC, attesting to a general involvement of the frontal lobe in this task. Some hemispheric lateralization to the right was found for HHb, whereas O2Hb and tHb showed their activation foci medially.

Fig. 6

Cortical activation during WM task: high-frequency signals. The activation maps are obtained from t-statistic values, assessed by a second-level analysis of the GLM model. From left to right: O2Hb, HHb, and tHb maps. (a) Each map depicts the significant difference between task condition and baseline. (b) Significant difference between the 0-back condition and baseline. (c) Significant difference between the 1-back condition and baseline. P<0.001 for all maps.

JBO_17_5_056005_f006.png

High-frequency components were then analyzed by means of DM2 [Fig. 2(b)] for a load-by-load signal analysis. Results seemed to suggest that high-frequency activity underwent a modulation induced by the task presentation sequence, thus following the alternation of task/rest blocks [Figs. 5 and 7(c)]. Owing to this characteristic, this component should be the one that mostly represents the real cortical activity related to the WM effort. In Fig. 6(b), the 0-back (pure attentive) condition is contrasted against baseline. Maps of O2Hb, HHb, and tHb are presented. HHb shows a strongly significant right lateralization. O2Hb and tHb maps attest to a different distribution of cerebral oxygenation over the PFC during the lightest period of the task, showing a peak of statistics medially.

Fig. 7

Second-level analysis of the GLM model: high-frequency signals. From left to right: O2Hb, HHb, and tHb maps. (a) Each map depicts the significant difference between the 2-back condition and baseline. (b) Each map depicts the significant difference between the 3-back condition and baseline. (c) Each map depicts the significant difference between the 1-back condition and 0-back. P<0.001 for all maps.

JBO_17_5_056005_f007.png

Figures 6(c), 7(a), and 7(b) show the pseudocolor representations of the t-statistic values of O2Hb, HHb, and tHb courses (high-frequency signals) for 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back loaded conditions, respectively, contrasted with the baseline. At any load, HHb shows a strong activation, considerably lateralized over the right hemisphere. O2Hb also shows activation, though more uniform and medially located. Spatial distribution of tHb shows a mild lateralization to the left, more pronounced in the 1-back and 3-back conditions.

The study of neighboring loads provided us with congruent results: contrasts between 0-back (pure attentional) and higher loads (working memory conditions) highlighted significant differences in activation pattern [see Fig. 7(c) for an example of activation pattern resulting from contrast between the pure attentional and the easy WM conditions]. Contrasts between different WM loads (1-back versus 2-back, 1-back versus 3-back, etc.) showed a trend of difference in activation patterns in the left hemisphere, though not significant.

GLM was finally run for low-frequency data (by means of both DM1 and DM2). Maps revealed a faint activation of O2Hb in the 1-back and 2-back conditions. Surprisingly, HHb proved to be significant at every task load, if contrasted with baseline. A poor right lateralization was still detected for HHb; though omnipresent, it showed no statistical significance.

During the employment of GLM, the variance of model residuals was calculated, for both DM1 and DM2. Table 1 shows our findings.

Table 1

Variances of the GLM residuals. From left to right, design matrices are listed, together with frequency contributions and values of the variance of GLM residuals for oxygenation, deoxygenation, and total hemoglobin signals. All values are obtained in the form of mean value and standard deviation from the whole population. In all cases, DM2 gave a better description of data, thus providing lower residuals.

DMContributionO2HbHHbtHb
DM1High frequency0.0730+0.01930.0160+0.00670.0477+0.0127
DM1Low frequency0.0121+0.01940.0031+0.00650.0071+0.0128
DM2High frequency0.0707+0.00790.0156+0.00270.0464+0.0045
DM2Low frequency0.0113+0.00790.0029+0.00270.0066+0.0045

4.

Discussion

4.1.

Methodological Issues

A limitation of the fNIRS technique is that signals contain contributions from both the brain and the overlying tissues.35 Specifically, the fNIRS signal can be highly sensitive to extracerebral blood volume changes. In addition, differences in the dynamics of O2Hb and HHb variations often reported in fNIRS studies have been generally interpreted as a potential indicator of differences in physiological mechanisms of neurovascular coupling across cortical areas.59 Surveys have also shown that this fact may indeed depend partly on hemodynamic changes unrelated to the cortical increase in blood flow. The most reliable hypothesis, then, is that this difference may be caused by systemic changes.60

In contrast to other neuroimaging methods and to the continuous-wave fNIRS, time-domain fNIRS can inherently differentiate between truly cortical and systemic changes, by exploiting depth information encoded in photon arrival times: on average, photons detected after long times of flight probe deeper tissue layers than early photons [35,37,38,42,43]. In doing so, a separation of surface and cortical contributions is made feasible. In the present work, extracerebral signal was isolated and treated separately to obtain neater information from the cortex. It was verified that extracerebral contribution gives no information significantly related with the task (data not shown). Further discussion on this topic is provided elsewhere.42

4.2.

Discussion of Results

Both pure attentional effort and engagement of executive functions (such as WM) are known to activate the dorsolateral PFC.24 In spite of this, attention and higher cognitive faculties differ phenomenologically, and cannot be regarded as the same process. Rather, attention is nowadays understood to serve as an underlying process on which the other cognitive activities can graft and develop. It is then possible that physiological differences existing between attention and higher processes are mirrored in some unlikeness in the dynamics and extension of PFC activation.9 The n-back task decouples attentional and mnemonic contributions to the global cognitive effort, and in addition allows the investigation of cortical activation during incremental cognitive exertion and, eventually, fatigue.

WM errors may arise from failing to encode the target, losing internal representations during maintenance, or incorrect coding of the target held in mind.27 The first case prevents the subject’s brain from entering the maintenance step of memory, potentially causing a lack of PFC activation with respect to correct trials; the third case is not affected by the risk of an absence of activation demand, generating an incorrect recollection; the second case clearly consists of a trade-off between the other two cases. The n-back task requires a constant encoding load: velocity of encoding never changes among conditions. The difficulty of the task is modulated by the maintenance request only. For this reason, if subjects perform correctly during the 0-back condition, a decay of PFC activation due to encoding performance can be excluded and—importantly—any change in PFC activation can be attributed to maintenance only. In our study, all subjects performed very well during the 0-back condition, and they proved to be able to afford even the most difficult (3-back) condition of the task.

fNIRS activation studies have demonstrated that cognitive tasks generally cause increases of O2Hb and tHb associated with decreases of HHb in the prefrontal cortex.6165 The increases of O2Hb and tHb reflect the occurrence of redundant regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) increases in response to neuronal activation, and the decrease of HHb is caused by evoked-rCBF rises that exceed the increases in oxygen consumption during neuronal activity.66 Our results agree with these previous findings, as shown in Fig. 4. It has been reported that cortical activation results in an overshoot of regional blood supply that is disproportionately larger than the increase in oxygen demand.66 This phenomenon is known as the physiological uncoupling of oxygen metabolism and blood supply. Augmented arterial blood flow into the cerebral cortex washes away deoxygenated hemoglobin more rapidly, resulting in a decrease in HHb with an increase in O2Hb.20,67 In this study, an explanation by the above mechanism is applicable.

The relative O2Hb concentration in frontal brain tissue increased significantly and bilaterally during the performance of the n-back test, compared with a resting condition. This result can be interpreted as an expression of increased cerebral perfusion as a consequence of an activated brain metabolism during the task. Simultaneously, HHb showed a progressive reduction. This latter effect is consistent with an enhanced HHb draining, secondary to hyperperfusion.

During a two-channel fNIRS registration of a continuous performance test in healthy subjects, a significant frontal O2Hb increase was found, compared with a baseline at rest, by Fallgatter and Strik.68 This result is generally interpreted as an enhanced perfusion of the frontal lobes consistent with attentive local activation. Lee et al.27 reported an increase for both O2Hb and HHb in a WM task performed by healthy people. Data of the present study seem not to support this evidence, as we found an increase in O2Hb with a concurrent decrease in HHb in the n-back task for WM assessment, as in Toichi and Kamio,69 who reported an increase in O2Hb with a concurrent decrease in HHb in tasks of higher cognitive function (i.e., WM). Furthermore, Lee et al.27 also observed an increase of both O2Hb and HHb in tasks of pure attention and a lack of increase in tHb in some subjects during tasks of higher cognitive processing, which we could not observe in our data. They hypothesized that the cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen consumption during tasks of attention may be higher than that during tasks of higher cognitive processing and, consistently with this interpretation, they proposed the change in HHb to be a physiological marker discriminating between attention and higher cognitive processing. The origin of such discrepancies can be found in the differences in the technologies employed: continuous wave NIRS, used in Toichi’s and Lee’s works, could provide data more affected by the superficial systemic contribution.

Our results agree with recent studies by Shibuya-Tayoshi et al.25 and Nakahachi et al.,26 who found a bilateral O2Hb increase prefrontally. Interestingly, a widespread, powerful right HHb activation was constantly present in GLM maps throughout the test. fNIRS literature can provide only partial confirmations of this result.

The analysis of contrasts between neighboring loads has revealed that 0-back (pure attentional) condition and higher load conditions (WM) elicit significantly different activation patterns in the PFC [Fig. 7(c), for example]. This is consistent with the difference between the two cognitive categories at a neural level. Also, contrasts between different WM loads (1-back versus 2-back, 1-back versus 3-back, etc.) showed differences in activation patterns that were maximal in the left hemisphere, though not significant; the left lateralization is consistent with previous results produced by Mayer et al.11 in an fMRI study, while the lack of significance between loads can relate to the scanty statistical power associated with the modulation of PFC activation, which is not comparable with a newly generated activation focus. However, as the attentive background is constant throughout the test of our study, the result appears to be specific for the demands of the augmented memory load, and not due to the general effects of attention, perception, and motor response. Linden et al.70 found that PFC responds to WM load beyond the capacity of the parietal-premotor network. Therefore, WM load-selective activation in the PFC might fit within the framework postulating that this brain region subserves extramnemonic processes of top-down control over posterior regions where information is actually stored.71,72 This is in agreement with the continuous increase of PFC activation through loads seen in our study.

Last, comparing Fig. 6(b) with Figs. 6(c), 7(a), and 7(b), it can clearly be seen that the overlap of the cerebral networks of WM and attention observed in the frontal midline by Mayer et al.11 likely finds confirmation in the present study. Figure 6(b), indeed, shows an O2Hb midfrontal pattern due to attention only, which matches the literature. Other confirmations can be found in Hopf et al.8 and Tomasi et al.9

During the use of GLM for the analysis of high- and low-frequency data, the variance of model residuals was calculated for both DM1 and DM2. Table 1 shows our findings. In all cases, DM2 gave a better description of data, thus providing lower residuals. This result confirms that a better modeling of data is obtained if the different conditions administered during the task are separately taken into account inside the linear model.

Last, we point out that, as we considered the presence of the interhemispheric fissure, some spatial undersampling occurred in the medial area, and thus we cannot completely rule out small shifts in localizing the medial activations due to spatial interpolation. This fact, albeit marginal, could be considered one limitation of our study.

5.

Conclusions

The present investigation by multichannel time-domain fNIRS supports results from fMRI studies, showing enhanced frontal lobe perfusion during performance of the n-back task. Overlapping activation for attention-demanding visual search and WM was observed in distributed frontal regions; nevertheless, the combined employment of time-resolved and GLM processing succeeded in identifying distinctive patterns of cortical activation for the two different neurophysiological categories under investigation (attention and short-term memory). Indeed, a subset of regions, in the right prefrontal cortex, showed an additive increase in O2Hb activation passing from attentional demand to WM effort.

In conclusion, the present study (1) successfully explored the capability of fNIRS (and more specifically time-domain fNIRS) to detect and characterize the prefrontal patterns of activation risen by attention and WM, and (2) contributed to highlight the advantages deriving from a multiple application of GLM to fNIRS data. On the other hand, a statistically significant distinction of cognitive load could not be reached in this study.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Michele Butti for the contribution given during the earliest part of the study. The research leading to these results has received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme under the nEUROPt Project (grant agreement FP7-HEALTH-2007-201076). The authors declare that no conflict of interest exists for the publication of this paper. They declare that this work has never been published previously. Authors all agree with the publication. If accepted, the work will not be published elsewhere in the same form, in English or other languages.

References

1. 

J. McAfooseB. T. Baune, “Exploring visual-spatial working memory: a critical review of concepts and models,” Neuropsychol. Rev., 19 (1), 130 –142 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-008-9063-0 NERVEJ Google Scholar

2. 

S. Funahashi, “Prefrontal cortex and working memory processes,” Neuroscience, 139 (1), 251 –261 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.07.003 NRSCDN 0306-4522 Google Scholar

3. 

T. Klingberg, “Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuospatial working memory,” Neuropsychologia, 44 (11), 2171 –2177 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.019 NUPSA6 0028-3932 Google Scholar

4. 

M. Petrides, “Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization,” Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B, 360 (1456), 781 –795 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1631 PTRBAE 0962-8436 Google Scholar

5. 

L. PessoaL. G. Ungerleider, “Top-down mechanisms for working memory and attentional processes,” The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 919 –930 MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (2004). Google Scholar

6. 

M. H. Munket al., “Distributed cortical systems in visual short-term memory revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging,” Cereb. Cortex, 12 (8), 866 –876 (2002). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/12.8.866 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

7. 

C. E. Curtis, “Prefrontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory,” Neuroscience, 139 (1), 173 –180 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.070 NRSCDN 0306-4522 Google Scholar

8. 

J. M. Hopfet al., “Direct neurophysiological evidence for spatial suppression surrounding the focus of attention in vision,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 103 (4), 1053 –1058 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507746103 PNASA6 0027-8424 Google Scholar

9. 

D. Tomasiet al., “Common deactivation patterns during working memory and visual attention tasks: an intra-subject fMRI study at 4 Tesla,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 27 (8), 694 –705 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

10. 

M. CorbettaJ. M. KincadeG. L. Shulman, “Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., 14 (4), 508 –523 (2002). http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892902317362029 JCONEO Google Scholar

11. 

J. S. Mayeret al., “Common neural substrates for visual working memory and attention,” Neuroimage, 36 (2), 441 –453 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.007 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

12. 

N. Axmacheret al., “Interactions between medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and inferior temporal regions during visual working memory: a combined intracranial EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” J. Neurosci., 28 (29), 7304 –7312 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1778-08.2008 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

13. 

J. RissmanA. GazzaleyM. D’Esposito, “Dynamic adjustments in prefrontal, hippocampal, and inferior temporal interactions with increasing visual working memory load,” Cereb. Cortex, 18 (7), 1618 –1629 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm195 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

14. 

S. M. Jaeggiet al., “Does excessive memory load attenuate activation in the prefrontal cortex? Load-dependent processing in single and dual tasks: functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” Neuroimage, 19 (2), 210 –225 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00098-3 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

15. 

M. Banajiet al., “A model of brain circulation and metabolism: NIRS signal changes during physiological challenges,” PLoS Comput. Biol., 4 (11), e1000212 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000212 1553-734X Google Scholar

16. 

F. F. Jobsis, “Non invasive, infrared monitoring of cerebral and myocardial oxygen sufficiency and circulatory parameters,” Science, 198 (4323), 1264 –1267 (1977). http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.929199 SCIEAS 0036-8075 Google Scholar

17. 

D. HightonC. ElwellM. Smith, “Noninvasive cerebral oximetry: is there light at the end of the tunnel?,” Curr. Opin. Anaesthesiol., 23 (5), 576 –581 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833e1536 COAEE2 Google Scholar

18. 

E. M. C. Hillman, “Optical brain imaging in vivo: techniques and applications from animal to man,” J. Biomed. Opt., 12 (5), 051402 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.2789693 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

19. 

H. ObrigA. Villringer, “Beyond the visible-imaging the human brain with light,” J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 23 (1), 1 –18 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004647-200301000-00001 JCBMDN 0271-678X Google Scholar

20. 

A. VillringerB. Chance, “Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function,” Trends Neurosci., 20 (10), 435 –442 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01132-6 TNSCDR 0166-2236 Google Scholar

21. 

M. WolfM. FerrariV. Quaresima, “Progress of near-infrared spectroscopy and topography for brain and muscle clinical applications,” J. Biomed. Opt., 12 (6), 062104 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.2804899 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

22. 

H. Itoet al., “Prefrontal overactivation, autonomic arousal, and task performance under evaluative pressure: a near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) study,” Psychophysiology, 48 (11), 1563 –1571 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.2011.48.issue-11 PSPHAF 0048-5772 Google Scholar

23. 

M. Sanefujiet al., “Strategy in short-term memory for pictures in childhood: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,” Neuroimage, 54 (3), 2394 –2400 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.090 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

24. 

T. Schreppelet al., “Activation of the prefrontal cortex in working memory and interference resolution processes assessed with near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neuropsychobiology, 57 (4), 188 –193 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000147473 NPBYAL 0302-282X Google Scholar

25. 

S. Shibuya-Tayoshiet al., “Activation of the prefrontal cortex during the trail-making test detected with multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy,” Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci., 61 (6), 616 –621 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01727.x 1323-1316 Google Scholar

26. 

T. Nakahachiet al., “Frontal cortex activation associated with speeded processing of visuospatial working memory revealed by multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy during Advanced Trail Making Test performance,” Behav. Brain Res., 215 (1), 21 –27 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.016 BBREDI 0166-4328 Google Scholar

27. 

J. Leeet al., “Origins of spatial working memory deficits in schizophrenia: an event-related FMRI and near-infrared spectroscopy study,” PLoS One, 3 (3), e1760 (2008). http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001760 1932-6203 Google Scholar

28. 

D. D. Jolleset al., “Practice effects in the brain: changes in cerebral activation after working memory practice depend on task demands,” NeuroImage, 52 (2), 658 –668 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.028 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

29. 

L. Nyberget al., “Neural correlates of variable working memory load across adult age and skill: dissociative patterns within the fronto-parietal network,” Scand. J. Psychol., 50 (1), 41 –46 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.2009.50.issue-1 SJPYA2 0036-5564 Google Scholar

30. 

B. RypmaJS. BergerM. D’Esposito, “The influence of working-memory demand and subject performance on prefrontal cortical activity,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., 14 (5), 721 –731 (2002). http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290260138627 JCONEO Google Scholar

31. 

R. Vandenbergheet al., “Attention to one or two features in left or right visual field: a positron emission tomography study,” J. Neurosci., 17 (10), 3739 –3750 (1997). JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

32. 

S. Puet al., “A multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy study of prefrontal cortex activation during working memory task in major depressive disorder,” Neurosci. Res., 70 (1), 91 –97 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.01.001 0976-8866 Google Scholar

33. 

V. Quaresimaet al., “Prefrontal cortex dysfunction during cognitive tests evidenced by functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Psychiatr. Res., 171 (3), 252 –257 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.02.002 PSRSDR Google Scholar

34. 

M. Schecklmannet al., “Prefrontal oxygenation during working memory in ADHD,” J. Psychiatr. Res., 44 (10), 621 –628 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.11.018 JPYRA3 0022-3956 Google Scholar

35. 

A. Liebertet al., “Time-resolved multidistance near-infrared spectroscopy of the adult head: intracerebral and extracerebral absorption changes from moments of distribution of times of flight of photons,” Appl. Opt., 43 (15), 3037 –3047 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/AO.43.003037 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

36. 

V. Quaresima, “Bilateral prefrontal cortex oxygenation responses to a verbal fluency task: a multichannel time-resolved near-infrared topography study,” J. Biomed. Opt., 10 (1), 011012 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.1851512 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

37. 

J. Selb, “Improved sensitivity to cerebral hemodynamics during brain activation with a time-gated optical system: analytical model and experimental validation,” J. Biomed. Opt., 10 (1), 11013 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.1852553 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

38. 

J. Steinbrinket al., “Determining changes in NIR absorption using a layered model of the human head,” Phys. Med. Biol., 46 (3), 879 –896 (2001). http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0031-9155/46/3/320 PHMBA7 0031-9155 Google Scholar

39. 

D. Continiet al., “Multi-channel time-resolved system for functional near infrared spectroscopy,” Opt. Express, 14 (12), 5418 –5432 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/OE.14.005418 OPEXFF 1094-4087 Google Scholar

40. 

Y. NomuraO. HazekiM. Tamura, “Relationship between time-resolved and non-time-resolved Beer-Lambert law in turbid media,” Phys. Med. Biol., 42 (6), 1009 –1023 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0031-9155/42/6/002 PHMBA7 0031-9155 Google Scholar

41. 

D. Continiet al., “Novel method for depth-resolved brain functional imaging by time-domain NIRS,” Proc. SPIE, 6629 662908 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/12.728104 Google Scholar

42. 

F. Alettiet al., “Deep and surface hemodynamic signal from functional time resolved transcranial near infrared spectroscopy compared to skin flowmotion,” Comput. Biol. Med., 42 (3), 282 –289 (2012). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compbiomed.2011.06.001 CBMDAW 0010-4825 Google Scholar

43. 

S. Del BiancoF. MartelliG. Zaccanti, “Penetration depth of light re-emitted by a diffusive medium: theoretical and experimental investigation,” Phys. Med. Biol., 47 (23), 4131 –4144 (2002). http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0031-9155/47/23/301 PHMBA7 0031-9155 Google Scholar

44. 

M. Buttiet al., “Effect of prolonged stimulation on cerebral hemodynamic: a time-resolved fNIRS study,” Med. Phys., 36 (9), 4103 –4114 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1118/1.3190557 MPHYA6 0094-2405 Google Scholar

45. 

R. A. JohnsonD. W. Wichern, Applied Multivariate Statistical Analysis, 5th ed.Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ (2002). Google Scholar

46. 

K. J. Fristonet al., “Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 2 (4), 189 –210 (1994). http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.v2:4 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

47. 

K. J. Friston, “Statistical parametric mapping,” Functional Neuroimaging, 79 –93 Academic Press, San Diego, CA (1994). Google Scholar

48. 

P. Moosmannet al., “Correlates of alpha rhythm in functional magnetic resonance imaging and near infrared spectroscopy,” NeuroImage, 20 (1), 145 –158 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00344-6 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

49. 

K. J. WorsleyK. J. Friston, “Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited—again,” NeuroImage, 2 (3), 173 –181 (1995). http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1995.1023 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

50. 

J. Cohen-Adadet al., “Activation detection in diffuse optical imaging by means of the general linear model,” Med. Image Anal., 11 (6), 616 –629 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.media.2007.06.002 Google Scholar

51. 

K. E. Janget al., “Wavelet-MDL detrending for near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS),” J. Biomed. Opt., 14 (3), 1 –13 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.3127204 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

52. 

M. M. Plichtaet al., “Model-based analysis of rapid event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) data: a parametric validation study,” NeuroImage, 35 (2), 625 –634 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.028 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

53. 

M. L. Schroeteret al., “Towards a standard analysis for functional near-infrared imaging,” NeuroImage, 21 (1), 283 –290 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.054 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

54. 

J. C. Yeet al., “NIRS-SPM: statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neuroimage, 44 (2), 428 –447 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

55. 

J. D. Cohenet al., “Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task,” Nature, 386 (6625), 604 –608 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/386604a0 NATUAS 0028-0836 Google Scholar

56. 

J. D. Cohenet al., “Activation of prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with fMRI,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 1 (4), 293 –304 (1993). http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.v1:4 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

57. 

A. R. Luria, Higher Cortical Functions in Man, Basic Books, New York, NY (1980). Google Scholar

58. 

G. D. Greenberg, Neuropsychological History Questionnaire, IDS Publishing, Worthington, OH (1994). Google Scholar

59. 

T. J. Huppertet al., “A temporal comparison of BOLD, ASL, and NIRS hemodynamic responses to motor stimuli in adult humans,” NeuroImage, 29 (2), 368 –382 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.065 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

60. 

D. A. BoasA. M. DaleM. A. Franceschini, “Diffuse optical imaging of brain activation: approaches to optimizing image sensitivity, resolution, and accuracy,” Neuroimage, 23 (Suppl. 1), S275 –S288 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.011 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

61. 

C. Hocket al., “Age dependency of changes in cerebral hemoglobin oxygenation during brain activation: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,” J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 15 (6), 1103 –1108 (1995). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/jcbfm.1995.137 JCBMDN 0271-678X Google Scholar

62. 

C. Hocket al., “Decrease in parietal cerebral hemoglobin oxygenation during performance of a verbal fluency task in patients with Alzheimer’s disease monitored by means of near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): correlation with simultaneous rCBF-PET measurements,” Brain Res., 755 (2), 293 –303 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00122-4 BRREAP 1385-299X Google Scholar

63. 

Y. HoshiM. Tamura, “Near-infrared optical detection of sequential brain activation in the prefrontal cortex during mental tasks,” Neuroimage, 5 (4 Pt. 1), 292 –297 (1997). http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1997.0270 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

64. 

Y. Hoshi, “Functional near-infrared optical imaging: utility and limitations in human brain mapping,” Psychophysiology, 40 (4), 511 –520 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.2003.40.issue-4 PSPHAF 0048-5772 Google Scholar

65. 

K. Sakataniet al., “Language-activated cerebral blood oxygenation and hemodynamic changes of the left prefrontal cortex in poststroke aphasic patients: a near-infrared spectroscopy study,” Stroke, 29 (7), 1299 –1304 (1998). http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.29.7.1299 SJCCA7 0039-2499 Google Scholar

66. 

P. T. FoxM. E. Raichle, “Focal physiological uncoupling of cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism during somatosensory stimulation in human subjects,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 83 (4), 1140 –1147 (1986). http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.83.4.1140 PNASA6 0027-8424 Google Scholar

67. 

Y. HoshiN. KobayashiM. Tamura, “Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model,” J. Appl. Physiol., 90 (5), 1657 –1662 (2001). JAPYAA 0021-8987 Google Scholar

68. 

A. J. FallgatterW. K. Strik, “Frontal brain activation during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test assessed with two-channel near-infrared spectroscopy,” Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci., 248 (5), 245 –249 (1998). EAPNES Google Scholar

69. 

M. ToichiY. Kamio, “Long-term memory in high-functioning autism: controversy on episodic memory in autism reconsidered,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 33 (2), 151 –161 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1022935325843 JADDDQ Google Scholar

70. 

D. E. J. Lindenet al., “Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network,” NeuroImage, 20 (3), 1518 –1530 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.021 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

71. 

C. E. CurtisM. D’Esposito, “Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 7 (9), 415 –423 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9 TCSCFK 1364-6613 Google Scholar

72. 

B. R. Postle, “Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain,” Neuroscience, 139 (1), 23 –38 (2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005 NRSCDN 0306-4522 Google Scholar
© 2012 Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers (SPIE) 0091-3286/2012/$25.00 © 2012 SPIE
Erika Molteni, Giuseppe Baselli, Sergio Cerutti, Anna Maria Bianchi, Davide Contini, Matteo Caffini, Rinaldo Cubeddu, Alessandro Torricelli, and Lorenzo Spinelli "Load-dependent brain activation assessed by time-domain functional near-infrared spectroscopy during a working memory task with graded levels of difficulty," Journal of Biomedical Optics 17(5), 056005 (4 May 2012). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.17.5.056005
Published: 4 May 2012
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 44 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS
Get copyright permission  Get copyright permission on Copyright Marketplace
KEYWORDS
Brain activation

Data modeling

Cognitive modeling

Statistical analysis

Near infrared spectroscopy

Brain

Brain mapping

Back to Top